Investigating the Psychological and Physiological Effects of Igusa (Juncus effusus L. var. decipiens “Suzukaze”) Scent: A Pilot Study Using Igusa Water
Seiko Goto1*, Hiroshi Hamano2, Taisuke Nakashima3, Fumi Kishida4, Akiko Isa5, Fadilla Zennifa6, Yanli Xu7, and Kuniyoshi Shimizu8
1Nagasaki University, School of Environmental Study, Nagasaki, 852-8521, Japan
2Hamano Hospital, Sasebo, 857-0016, Japan
3-8Kyushu University, Faculty of Agriculture, Fukuoka, 819-0395, Japan
*Corresponding Author: Seiko Goto, Nagasaki University, School of Environmental Study, Nagasaki, 852-8521, Japan
Received: 09 November 2024; Accepted: 12 November 2024; Published: 02 December 2024;
Article Information
Citation:
Seiko Goto, Hiroshi Hamano, Taisuke Nakashima, Fumi Kishida, Akiko Isa, Fadilla Zennifa, Yanli Xu, and Kuniyoshi Shimizu. Investigating the Psychological and Physiological Effects of Igusa (Juncus effusus L. var. decipiens “Suzukaze”) Scent: A Pilot Study Using Igusa Water. Journal of Environmental Science and Public Health. 8 (2024): 175-185.
View / Download Pdf Share at FacebookAbstract
Background: Tatami mats, an integral part of traditional Japanese architecture, are renowned for their distinctive scent primarily derived from rush (Juncus effusus L. var. decipiens) or igusa in Japanese. Although the aroma of tatami is deeply ingrained in Japanese culture, its potential psychological and physiological effects remain underexplored. Methods: This study investigated the effects of igusa scent on mood, cognitive function, and behavior. The experiment was conducted in three stages to clarify its effects; 1. on brain activity when individuals inhaled a scent released from an inhaler in a small closed room, 2. on healthy subjects’ moods in a normal living environment with a scent emitted by a diffuser, and 3. on the well-being of elderly individuals in a normal living environment using scents emitted by a diffuser. EEG monitor, visual stimuli tests, POMS mood test, 100 square calculation tests, and questionnaire were used for healthy individuals. MMSE test, DBD test, and CMAI test were used for individuals with dementia. Results: Subsequent experiments involving healthy individuals and elderly participants in nursing homes revealed that igusa scent enhanced cognitive function, improved mood, and reduced behavioral symptoms, especially among elderly individuals with dementia. Conclusions: The scent of igusa water demonstrated its effects regardless of preference. It can also be expected to have an effect through long-term exposure, even for those who do not perceive the scent.
Keywords
Igusa scent, Tatami, 1,8-cineole, Aromatherapy, Cognitive function.
Igusa scent articles; Tatami articles; 1,8-cineole articles; Aromatherapy articles; Cognitive function articles
Article Details
1. Background
Tatami mats are traditional Japanese flooring mats made of rush (Juncus effusus L. var. decipiens), or igusa in Japanese (Figure 1). It has been reported that extracts from Juncus effusus have anti-inflammatory, antioxidant, and hemostatic effects, in addition to its protective nature against D-galactosamine-induced liver damage by decreasing oxidative stress [1].In Japan, tatami mats were initially used for aristocrats' seating and bedding during the 8th century, however in the later period, they were set from wall to wall and such a room became an essential element in Japanese architecture by the 16th century. Japanese rooms with tatami mats are characterized by their unique scent and appearance. The grassy scent of tatami mats, mainly from igusa, is highly regarded in Japanese culture and is often associated with a sense of comfort, relaxation, and nostalgia [2]. Although Western-style flooring is available in modern Japan, traditional Japanese rooms with tatami mats are still prevalent.

Figure 1: A typical example of a Japanese tatami room
To investigate the effects of the tatami mat scent, Sun et al. evaluated the effect of the scent of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) from Juncus effusus L. var. decipiens Buchen, dried igusa, and found an increase in alpha waves when subjects smelled the scent [3]. As the alpha band amplitude can be an indicator of the change in arousal level, this study showed that the scent of dried igusa has the effect of awakening the brain [4,5,6]. However, the specific effects of the scent of igusa on mood and behavior have not been conclusively proven. Moreover, the strength and quality of tatami mat’s scent varies over time, yet it is not clear whether and how the effect of the scent changes over time.
As recent lifestyle changes have led to a significant decrease in Japanese-style rooms with tatami mats, there are fewer opportunities for exposure to the tatami scent in daily life, even in Japan. If the scent of tatami mats has psychological or physiological improvement effects, the application of the igusa scent in a portable form, such as an aroma diffuser would be advantageous. As a large portion of igusa is cut off during the manufacture of tatami mats, and the amount of discarded igusa is causing a serious environmental problem in Japan, the conversion of discarded igusa into value-added products is highly desirable, even from the environmental protection purposes. As igusa does not produce essential oils from distillation, this study used Juncus effusus L. var. decipiens buchen hydrosol, igusa water to verify the effect of igusa scent in a portable form, and not as traditional tatami mats.
Using igusa water manufactured by Daiichi Food Industry [7], made by distilling steam from boiling igusa, a rush cultivar (Juncus effusus L. var. decipiens “Suzukaze”), harvested in Yatsushiro, Kumamoto city, Japan. This study explored the scent effects of its VOCs on healthy young individuals and elderly individuals in nursing homes. The experiment was conducted in three stages to clarify its effects; 1. on brain activity when individuals inhaled a scent released from an inhaler in a small closed room, 2. on healthy subjects’ moods in a normal living environment with a scent emitted by a diffuser, and 3. on the well-being of elderly individuals in a normal living environment using scents emitted by a diffuser. Prior to conducting the three experiments, a gas chromatography-mass spectrometry (GC-MS) analysis was performed using a 7890 A GC system (5975 C MSD) manufactured by Agilent Technologies with a thermal desorption unit to determine the VOCs in igusa water. The column used for analysis was VF-624MS (0.25 mm I.D. x60m, film thickness 1.4 µm). Column temperature was controlled as follows: 50oC (1 min) →160oC (15oC/min) → 250oC (5oC/min). Helium was used as the carrier gas at a flow rate of 1.5 mL/min, and the analysis mode was SCAN. The compounds were identified by Aroma Office Ver.5.0 with the NIST08MassSpectral Library Database. As a result of the analysis, the main VOCs detected in igusa water were Hexanal, D-Limonene, and 1,8-cineole (Figure 2).

Figure 2: Volatile organic compounds (VOCs) of igusa water identified using chromatograms in the GS-MS analysis
2. Materials and Methods
Experiment 1. The effects on brain activity
The purpose of this study was to confirm the effect of igusa water on brain activity using an EEG monitoring and visual discrimination task. All participants (9 males, 8 females) were prohibited from consuming alcohol or medications one day before the experiment, and caffeine on the day of the experiment. For the experiment, a sound proofed room (1.2 m × 1.7 m × 2.0 m) equipped with an inhaler connected to Tenax glass tubes to provide the scent of igusa water from a diffuser was prepared (Figure 3 18). The CRT display was placed 114 cm from the subject’s seat in the room. To clean the air in the room before the experiment, it was first ventilated using an exhaust fan and by opening the door 15 min before the experiment. Subsequently, the exhaust fan was stopped, and the door was closed for 30 min with a small fan circulating the air. The mini-pumps used to release the scent were activated 10 min before the participants entered the room and stopped after completion of their tasks. Each participant entered the experiment room after providing written informed consent, attached the EEG monitor, and played six sessions of 2.5 min visual stimuli tests while viewing the monitor and smelling the scent released from the inhaler.

Figure 3: Experimental equipment and arrangement for Experiment 1
Experiment 2. The effects to the mood of healthy individuals
The purpose of this experiment was to investigate the effect of igusa water’s scent in a living room, using distilled water and 1,8-cineole as controls. The room used for the experiment was a regular classroom approximately 5 m × 6 m in size with windows (Figure 4 19). Two essential oil diffusers were used to diffuse the scent of 1,8-cineole, and two ultrasonic humidifiers were used to diffuse igusa water (diluted 10 times with distilled water), and distilled water respectively. The diffusing device was placed diagonally at a corner of the room (two devices in total). The room was ventilated prior to the experiment. Thirty minutes after the start of the experiment, the fan was turned off, windows were closed, and the diffusers were turned on. An air sample of 1.8-cineole was collected at the center of the room 30 min after diffusion with an air collection pump for 15 s and an air flow rate of 0.15 L/min, measuring 889.73 μg/m³. In contrast, the VOCs of air sample of igusa water were hexanal: 58.8 μg/m³, D-limonene: 83.2 μg/m³, and 1,8-cineole: 259.7 μg/m³.

Figure 4: A typical room used for Experiment 2
Experiment 3. The effects to elderly individuals’ well-being
The purpose of this study was to investigate the long-term exposure effects of igusa water on the elderly population. A diffuser or humidifier was placed near the bed and turned on for one hour at bedtime for seven consecutive nights. For 1,8-cineole diffusion, eight drops of 1,8-cineole were used, which automatically stopped the aroma diffuser after approximately one hour. For igusa water diffusion, igusa water (diluted 10 times with distilled water) was used. An air sample of 1.8-cineole was collected at the center of the room 1 h after diffusion with an air collection pump for 15 s with an air flow rate of 0.15 L/min, measuring 1086.44 μg/m³. In contrast, the sample of igusa water VOCs had hexanal: 47.2 μg/m³, D-limonene: 99.9 μg/m³ and 1.8-cineole: 393.7 μg/m³.
Diffuser
For all experiments in this study, a 300 mL ultrasonic humidifier (Figure 5-A)22 was used to diffuse igusa water and distilled water, while an essential oil diffuser (Fig. 5-B)23 with a 2-min on, 1-min off cycle was used to diffuse 1,8-cineole.

Figure 5: Ultrasonic humidifier (A) and essential oil diffuser (B) used for experiments
Statistical analysis
In all results, the values are expressed as the means ± s.e.m. Paired t-tests with a Bonferroni collection were used to compare differences in each index between the conditions. Statistical significance was recognized when the p value was < 0.05, and the tendency was p < 0.1. All statistical analyses were performed using JASP 0.16.4 (JASP Team, 2022).
3. Results
Experiment 1. The effects on brain activity
Experiment 1 evaluated the effects of igusa water scent compared to that of distilled water (used as control), which has little odor. The experiment was conducted with nine healthy males and eight female young individuals using a questionnaire to assess mood and subjective impression of the scent. In addition, a visual stimuli test with an electroencephalograph (EEG) monitor was used to assess changes in work efficiency and brain waves caused by the scent. EEG is a non-invasive technique with excellent temporal resolution that is highly sensitive to fluctuations in human brain activity [8]. In the experiment, the subjects two sets of six sessions of visual stimuli tests, of 2.5 min each were performed on the subjects. One set smelled the scent of igusa water, and the other set smelled the scent of distilled water. Ultrasonic humidifiers were used to diffuse igusa water, which was diluted 100 times with distilled water and to diffuse distilled water. The VOC analysis conducted on samples collected from igusa water using an air collection pump for 15 s with an air flow rate of 0.15 L/min showed presence of Hexanal: 420 μg/m³, D-Limonene: 661 μg/m³, and 1,8-Cineol: 227 μg/m³.
The visual stimuli test was conducted to assess the level of participants’ focused attention to detail using a software “Presentation” by Neuro Behavioral System. In this test, three patterns of grayscale sinusoidal grating images - vertically straight pattern (T), tilted at 10 ° pattern (F), and tilted at 50 °pattern (I) appeared randomly on the screen. (T) appears with a probability of 15%, (F) with a probability of 70%, and (I) with a probability of 15%. The interstimulus intervals were also randomized between 850 and 1100 ms to make it difficult for the participants to anticipate. Participants were asked to press the button when (T) appeared (Figure 6). The task involved pressing a button accurately and quickly in response to (T). In addition to the visual stimuli test, a shortened version of the Profile Of Mood States (POMS) questionnaire was performed before and after the test, and a questionnaire survey was conducted to examine the impression of the scent after the test (Figure 7) [9]. The POMS questionnaire categorizes mood into six dimensions: "tension," "depression," "anger," "fatigue," "confusion," and "vigor." In addition, scores of each dimension were calculated to indicate negative mood states.

Figure 6: Visual test process

Figure 7: Experimental procedure of Experiment 1
Individuals recognized the scent of igusa water much stronger than that of the distilled water (2.00 ± 0.411 vs.0.824 ± 0.231, p = 0.048); however, preference for the scent of igusa water was lower than that of water (0.118 ± 0.169 vs. 0.412 ± 0.193, p = 0.289) (Figure 8). The results of the POMS questionnaire administered before and after the visual stimuli test did not show any significant differences compared to the distilled water condition. In contrast, toe results of the visual stimuli test, although no significant effect was found in the accuracy rate with the scent of igusa water (Figure 9), the reaction time was significantly faster with the scent of igusa water than that of the distilled water in session 3 (510.44 ± 16.35 msec vs. 561.82 ± 16.40, p = 0.01). Furthermore, EEG monitoring during the visual stimuli task displayed that gamma power during the task with the scent of igusa water was significantly larger than the distilled water (46.80 ± 16.08 vs. 39.34 ± 17.02, p = 0.016) (Figure 10). Gamma waves are known to appear during a state of high arousal (emotional and cognitive processing), and it can be said that the scent of igusa water enhance es sensory and cognitive processing functions [10].

Figure 8: Results of evaluation for the strength (left) and preference (right) of the scent

Figure 9: Results of visual tests: accuracy (left) and preference (right)

Figure 10: Changes of EEG gamma power in the visual tests
Experiment 2. Effects to healthy individuals’ mood
Experiment 2 examined the effects of the igusa water scent emitted by a diffuser in a normal living environment on 40 healthy young college students (20 males and 20 females, with an average age of 21.3 years). In addition to distilled water, 1,8-cineole was used as a control, which has been confirmed to have an arousing effect in previous studies [11]. The scent of 1,8-cineole is described as a fresh, with slightly minty aroma, and is often used in aromatherapy for its relaxing properties. The participants were randomly divided into two groups, A and B, for a single-blind experiment in three environments: a room with the scents of distilled water, 1,8-cineole, and igusa water. Both groups performed the experiment once a day using one scent. Experiments with different scents were performed at least three days apart. Both groups undertook the experiment in a room with the scent of distilled water first, subsequently followed by group A undertaking the experiment in a room with the scent of igusa water first, and group B undertaking the experiment in a room with the scent of 1,8-cineole first.
The experiment used 1) a survey to evaluate the preference for the scent on a 5-point scale, measuring the liking or disliking of the scent, 2) a shortened version of the POMS questionnaire to measure mood changes, and 3) 100 square calculation tests to measure the change in mood owing to the scent. This was an arithmetic task involving single-digit additions to a 10 × 10 grid within a limited time of 1 min. In addition, the number of correct answers were also recorded (Figure 11). In one experiment, the participants first completed the POMS questionnaire and 100 square calculation tests in a room with open windows that did not diffuse any scent. After completing the tests, they moved to the next room, where the scent was diffused. After they sat down and had a quiet time of 3 min, they completed a questionnaire about their scent preference, a calculation test, and a POMS questionnaire (Figure 12).

Figure 11: A sample of 100 square test.

Figure 12: Experimental procedure of Experiment 2.
Figure 13 presents the results of the questionnaire regarding scent preferences. Approximately 45% of participants very liked (18%) or liked (27%) the scent of 1,8-cineole, whereas none very liked (14 %) the scent of igusa water. In contrast, 15% of the participants very disliked (11%) or disliked (4%) the scent of 1,8-cineole, whereas 48% of the participants very disliked (9%) or disliked (39%) the scent of igusa water. The smell of igusa water reminded the participants of old barns, countryside, and grass, and was less favored than 1,8-cineole. The results of the 100 square calculation tests displayed that the average score significantly improved after exposure to 1,8-cineole (p=0.0046). In particular, all participants who answered that they disliked the igusa water scent performed worse in the 100 square calculation tests after exposure to the scent. However, despite the lower scent preference, igusa water demonstrated better results in the total mood disturbance (TMD) evaluation of the POMS questionnaire. The TMD was calculated by summing the scores of the first five subscales (tension-anxiety, depression-dejection, anger-hostility, fatigue-inertia, and confusion-bewilderment) and subtracting the vigor-activity score. TMD provides an overall measure of mood disturbance, with higher scores indicating greater negative affect. The results of TMD in this experiment indicated that the scent of igusa water had a greater mood-enhancing effect than 1,8-cineole among young individuals.

Figure 13: Preference of the scent of igusa water.
Experiment 3. The effects to the elderly subjects’ well-being
Experiment 3 was conducted with the residents of two nursing homes, WR (Wakaba Residence) [12] and WT (Wakaba Terrace), [13] in Sasebo City, Nagasaki Prefecture, Japan. 34 participants (WR: 11 females, 3 males; WT1:10 females; WT2:10 females) were recruited from residents who had mini-mental state examination (MMSE) scores between 10 and 27. The average age of participants was 92 years. Distilled water and 1,8-cineole were used as control. The experiment involved exposing the participants to one of the scents for 1 h before bedtime for seven consecutive days. As the participants' room doors were closed at bedtime, the scent components diffused by the diffuser lingered in the room while the participants were sleeping. All participants lived in a single room (approximately 3 m x 5 m) furnished with a bed, bedside table, closet, and a shelf (Figure 14). Temperature was controlled with air conditioning, and the windows were typically kept closed.

Figure 14: An example of a typical participant’s room.
The MMSE, dementia behavior disturbance scale (DBD), and Cohen-Mansfield agitation inventory (CMAI) were used as indicators to measure cognitive abilities and behavioral observations [14,15,16,17]. The MMSE is a questionnaire used to assess cognitive impairment by examining functions such as attention, calculation, recall, and language. It consists of 11 questions measuring cognitive ability. The DBD is a scale comprising 28 items that caregivers use to assess behavioral symptoms. The CMAI is a scale divided into aggressive behavior (11 items) and non-aggressive behavior (11 items), totaling 22 items, in which caregivers assess behavioral symptoms. The duration of the experiment was 6 weeks. The participants were exposed to scents for seven days during the 2nd and 5th week. The WT2 participants were exposed to the scent of distilled water for both weeks; while WT1 participants were exposed to igusa water during week 2 and 1,8-cineole during week 5; whereas WR participants were exposed to the scent of 1,8-cineole during week 2 and igusa water during week 5 (Figure15). None of the participants recognized the scents; however, all caregivers perceived the scents of 1,8-cineole and igusa water. During the 6 weeks of the experiment, four MMES tests were performed: week1 (before the first scent test), week 3 (after the first scent test), week 4 (before the second scent test), and week 6 (after the second scent test). Evaluation of DBD and CMAI was performed daily between 2 PM and 5 PM during weeks 2 and 5 respectively.

Figure 15: Experimental procedure of Experiment 3.
The effects on cognitive function
Figure 16 illustrates the changes in the normalized average MMSE scores of the three groups of participants (WR, WT1, and WT2) before and after the two exposure sessions. Although scent was not perceived by any participant, the MMSE score improved during the scent exposure periods. The effect of the scents is evident from the results that the average MMSE of WR and WT1 participants increased during the period of exposure and decreased during the 2 weeks of washout period. Although the caregivers preferred the smell of 1,8-cineol, the scent of igusa water had a more positive impact on the MMSE results compared to that of the scent of 1,8-cineole. The WR participants showed significantly improved MMSE scores with the scent of 1,8-cineole (p=0.02) and igusa water (p=0.001), while W1 participants showed a trend of improvement with the scents of igusa water (p=0.34) and 1,8-cineole (p=0.33). No significant change was observed in the MMSE scores of WT2 participants exposed to the scent of distilled water.

Figure 16: Normalized average MMSE between week 2 and week 6.
The effects on behavior
Figure 17 shows the average DBD and CMAI scores on day 1 (blue) and day 7 (red) of scent exposure. An increase in the values indicated an increase in behavioral issues. Comparing the results of days 1 and 7 of scent exposure, there was no significant change in the case of water; however, there was a significant improvement in DBD and CMAI for both igusa water (DBD, p=0.0005; CMAI, p=0.002) and 1,8-cineole (DBD, p=0.025; CMAI, p=0.013). In addition, the degree of improvement was greater for igusa water than for 1,8-cineole. While considering the details of the improvements, there was a particular improvement in items such as “trying to go to a different place,” “repeating the same actions,” “repeating the same words,” “urinary incontinence,” and “fecal incontinence.” According to caregivers’ records, there was an increase in requests from participants to use the toilet during the exposure period to igusa water and 1,8-cineole. These improvements could be because the scent had an effect on awakening the participants' consciousness.

Figure 17: Change in DBD and CMAI between day1 and day 7.
4. Discussion
As the Japanese usually favor the scent of new tatami, many participants disliked the scent of igusa water used for this experiment as it smelled like old tatami mats. Despite its unpopularity, Experiment 1 confirmed that the scent of igusa water triggered gamma waves in the brain. Experiment 3 displayed that the scent of igusa water improved the DBD, CMAI, and MMSE scores in elderly participants, although they were unaware of the scent. These results suggest that regardless of the perception of the scent, the scent of igusa water has awakening effects. These effects helped improve the cognitive function and behavioral symptoms of elderly participants with dementia and reduced the physical and psychological burden on caregivers. However, Experiment 2 found that the results of the 100 square calculation tests worsened for participants who found the scent unpleasant.
Generally, individual preferences for scents influence the effectiveness of aromatherapy, and it is important to consider individual preferences for scents for optimal effectiveness [18]. If the scent of igusa water had been pleasant, better effects could be expected. Therefore, Daiichi Food Company was requested to revise its manufacturing methods to improve its scent. After months of trial and error, a new igusa water was prepared by steaming igusa instead of boiling it. With this new igusa water, Experiment 2 included 40 healthy participants (20 males, 20 females), and Experiment 3 included 29 participants from the same nursing home (13 females from WR, 1 male from WR, and 15 females from WT) with an average age of 93 years. Distilled water was not used, however only 1,8-cineole was used as the control.
The scent of the new igusa water was similar to the scent of new tatami mats. The results of the survey evaluating the preference for the scent on a 5-point scale, measuring the liking or disliking of the scent, showed that the preference for the scent of new igusa water was not significantly different from that of 1,8-cineole (Figure 18). Although the results cannot be directly compared between the new igusa water and old igusa water as the subjects who participated in the experiment were different, none of the participants very liked, and only 14% of participants liked the scent of old igusa water, while 9% of participants very liked and 18% liked the new igusa water. However, the average accuracy rate of the 100-square calculation task significantly improved after exposure to 1,8-cineole (p=0.047), whereas the accuracy rate after the exposure to the new igusa water significantly decreased (p=0.015). In contrast, in the POMS mood test, TMD significantly improved with both scents (1,8-cineole, p=0.002; new igusa water, p=0.03). Therefore, although the impression rating of the scent of igusa water improved, the results of the 100-square calculation task and POMS did not improve.

Figure 18: Preference of the scent of new igusa water.
In nursing homes, as in the previous experiment, the elderly participants did not recognize either scent; however, the caregivers sensed both scents. However, there were no complaints from caregivers about the scent of the new igusa water. Figure19-A illustrates the average MMSE scores on days 1 and 7 after one week of exposure to the two scents. The MMSE score of the participants significantly improved with the scent of new igusa water (p=0.04), however it was not the case with the scent of 1,8-cineole. Positive effects on behavior (DBD and CMAI) were also observed for the two scents, as in the previous experiment. However, where significant improvements in the behavior of DBD (p=0.0001) and CMAI (p=0.003) were observed with 1,8-cineole, and only a trend toward improvement in DBD (p=0.3) and CMAI (p=0.1) was observed with new igusa water (Figure 19-B). As both 1,8-cineole and old igusa water significantly improved both DBD and CMAI, the effect of new igusa water on 1,8-cineole was not as good as that of the old igusa water on 1,8-cineole despite a better preference for the scent.

Figure 19: Change in MMSE between week 2 and week 6 (A) change in DBD and CMAI (B).
Figure 20 displays the chromatograms of air captured from the old and new igusa waters. The main VOCs in both air types were identified as hexanal, D-limonene, and 1,8-cineole with similar peaks [19]. Although the composition of VOCs between the old and new igusa waters are similar, the results of the VOC analysis that was collected from the old igusa water with an air collection pump for 15 s and an air flow rate of 0.15 L/min, included hexanal: 420 μg/m³, D-limonene: 661 μg/m³, and 1,8-cineol: 227 μg/m³. However, the results of the new igusa water showed the presence of hexanal: 266 μg/m³, D-limonene: 430 μg/m³, and 1,8-cineol: 115 μg/m³. As the new igusa water had approximately 63% hexanal, 65% D-limonene, and 50% 1,8-cineole compared to the old igusa water, the reason for the sensory difference in the scent of the old and new igusa water could be owing to the differences in the concentration of the components. Many participants felt that the scent of the new igusa water was better because it had a lower concentration of VOCs. Furthermore, there are limitations and challenges in identifying VOCs with GC-MS analysis. Although GC-MS analysis is a powerful technique for identifying and quantifying compounds in air, as igusa water contains a wide range of compounds, both organic and inorganic, some components may be undetectable in the analysis process. Humans are sensitive to scents and can judge whether the scents are different depending on the concentration including undetected microscopic elements of VOCs, even if the main components of the VOCs are the same.

Figure 20: Chromatograms of the air from old igusa water (A) and new igusa water(B).
5. Conclusion
In research on the relationship between scent preferences and relaxing effects, Akiyoshi stated that some aromas affect mood depending on the degree of preference, whereas others affect mood regardless of the degree of preference [20]. Although the scents of the old and new igusa water were recognized differently, the proportion of the main VOCs was similar, and similar effects were obtained regardless of the degree of preference. Whether the scent represented a new or an old tatami, the scent of igusa water improved the mood of both healthy young people who could detect the scents well as well as elderly people who could not detect the scents. Although the quality of the scent of tatami varied with age, it is generally considered to offer a sense of security. This study confirms this empirically cultivated perception. The scent of igusa water demonstrated its effects regardless of preference, however it can also be expected to have an effect through long-term exposure, even for those who do not perceive the scent [21].
If the scent of igusa could be used in a variety of places, including residences, offices, hospitals, and nursing homes, it would help to improve the quality of daily life. In particular, its effects on improvements in peripheral behavior could lead to reduced mental burden on caregivers and enhancement in quality of care. However, further research is required before igusa water can be used in practical applications. First, it is important to further investigate the effects of each VOC as well as the effects based on different distributions of VOCs, as well as the appropriate concentration and time of exposure. Second, the quality of aroma water can vary greatly depending on the quality of the plant and the extraction techniques used. It is necessary to continue trial and error to determine the best manufacturing method to obtain favorable and effective scents. Third, although this study was conducted with Japanese subjects who know the culture of tatami mats, further research is needed to determine whether the effects of the scent of igusa water depend on the cultural background.
Declarations
Acknowledgements
We would like to express our deepest appreciation to Ms. Aya Furusato and Ms.Yuko Takano for their invaluable contribution to the experiments of this study. We would also like to thank all the nursing staff at Hamano Hospital for their great cooperation in conducting experiments at the facility.
Author contributions
1.Designed the study, directed the projects, and wrote the manuscript.
2.Arranged and supervised the experiments at Wakaba Terrace and Wakaba Residence.
3.Planned the protocol for the EEG experiment, conducted data analysis, and summarized the data.
4.Conducted data analysis.
5.Conducted GC-MS analysis.
6.Conducted data analysis.
7.Conducted GC-MS analysis.
8.Designed the study and directed the project.
Ethical Approval
Experiment 1 was approved by Kyushu University Ethics Review Committee in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (approval #83, 2021/08/06), Experiment 2 and Experiment 3 were approved by Nagasaki University Ethics Review Committee in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (approval #R4-1, 2021/06/27, approval #R4-2, 2021/07/17).
Funding
This study was supported by Japan Rush Grass Industry Council and Grants-in Aid for Scientific Research (21K11082).
Data availability statement
The data from human experiments that support the findings of this study are not publicly available as they contain information that could compromise the privacy of research participants. Available data is provided within the manuscript.
Consent to publish statement
The image clipping used in Figure 18 obtained the consent for publication.
References
- Wang X, Zhao M, Ju C, et al. Protective mechanisms of juncus effusus and carbonized juncus effusus against D-galactosamine-induced acute liver injury in mice. Chem Pharm Bull 72 (2024): 280-285.
- Ito R. Imai, N & Kawamura M. An investigation of dwelling style and lifestyle related to Tatami rooms. 55th Annual Congress of the Japan Society of Home Economics. Conference proceeding 10 (2003): 228.
- Sun M, Nakashima T, Yoshimura Y, et al. Physiological and psychological effects of volatile organic compounds from dried common rush (juncus effusus L. var. decipiens Buchen.) on humans. Int J Environ Res Public Health 19 (2022): 1856.
- Ota T, Toyoshima R, Yamauchi T. Measurement of biphasic changes of the alpha band amplitude as indicator of arousal level. Int J psychophysiol 24 (1996): 25-37.
- Davis H, Davis PA. Action potentials of the brain in normal persons and in normal states of cerebral activity. Arch Neurol Psychiatry 36 (1936): 1214-1224.
- Okamoto T, Tamura K, Miyamoto N, et al. Physiological activity in calm thermal indoor environments. Sci Rep 7 (2017): 1-12.
- https://aroma.ski/view/company
- Ismail LE, Karwowski W. Applications of EEG indices for the quantification of human cognitive performance. A systematic review and bibliometric analysis.15 (2020): e0242857-e0242857.
- Curran S, Andrykowski MA, Studts JL. Short Form of the Profile of Mood States (POMS-SF): Psychometric Information. Psychol Assess 7 (1955): 80-83.
- Luo Q, Mitchell D, Cheng X, et al. Visual awareness, emotion, and gamma band synchronization, Cereb. Cortex 19 (2009): 1896-1904.
- Goto S, Suzuki H, Nakagawa T, et al. The Effect of Eucalyptol on Nursing Home Residents. Sci Rep 10 (2020): 4-10.
- https://www.satsuki-jutaku.jp/search/detail.php?house_id=5347
- https://www.wakaba-terrace.jp/
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. Mini-Mental State: a Practical Method for Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. J Psychiatric Res 12 (1975): 189-198.
- Baumgarten M, Becker R, Gauthier S. Validity and reliability of the dementia behavior disturbance scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 38 (1990): 221-226.
- Yamaguchi H, Nakajima T, Uchid H, et al. Evaluation of behavioral disturbance with dementia behavior disturbance (DBD) scale in the outpatients of the Medical Center for Dementia. Dementia Japan 31 (2017): 389-397.
- Cohen-Mansfield, J. Conceptualization of Agitation: Results Based on the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory and the Agitation Behavior Mapping Instrument. Int Psychogeriatrics 8 (1997):.309-315.
- Okano S, Yokoe A, Itsuse K, et al. Effects of Pre-nap Odor Presentation on Autonomic Nervous System Activity. Sleep Environ 15 (2020): 3-9 (2020).
- Yoshii F. nioi kagi GCM wo mochiita Shichitoui no kokiseibun bunseki. Memoirs of Beppu University 58 (2017): 137-146.
- Akiyoshi K. The relax effect which the degree of the likes and dislikes of the aroma gives to man. GINMU 9 (2013): 23-31.
- Furusato A, Goto S. A pilot study on the subtle scent effects of 1,8-cineole for cognitive function of elderly population. J Japanese Society Aromatherapy 21 (2022): 21-30.
- https://www.muji.com/jp/ja/store/cmdty/detail/4550583648824?sc_cid=google_pla_pj_direct_cp_14797768044_adg_151183754878_cr_none_opt_none&gad_source=1 &gclid=Cj0KCQjw_qexBhCoARIsAFgBleuBW-e_cR8kSMXxFfFbn1VCbWISPaqRXWU0INBwCcPYA0JtsEON7yEaAlZKEALw_wcB
- https://onlineshop.treeoflife.co.jp/collections/oil-diffuser/products/088010050